Does climate change concern alter individual tax preferences? Evidence from an Italian survey

Alessandro Cascavilla (University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy) (Universitat Jaume I, Castellón, Spain)

Journal of Economic Studies

ISSN: 0144-3585

Article publication date: 20 January 2023

Issue publication date: 22 November 2023

2731

Abstract

Purpose

This paper analyzes the role that the climate change concern (CCCi) has on the willingness to accept an environmental tax. The author aims to grasp how individual general tax preferences can differ with respect to the specific (environmental) tax. He focuses attention to the Italian case since it has been argued that the potential acceptability of a carbon tax in Italy is relatively high, and this topic has been scarcely explored so far among Italian citizens (Rotaris and Danielis, 2019).

Design/methodology/approach

The author conducted an online survey among 514 Italian economics students.

Findings

The CCCi positively influences the environmental tax morale (ETMi). The general tax morale (TMi) positively affects the specific (environmental) TMi. The CCCi alters individual tax preferences. The author evidenced that also subjects with low TMi turned out to be willing to pay an environmental tax if aware of the environmental issues.

Research limitations/implications

Although the author used a common methodology in this strand of research, he is aware that in an online survey individuals can be influenced by the self-reporting and hypothetical choice bias (see Swamy et al., 2001), that in turn can characterize their reported preferences. Moreover, even if economics university students are commonly used as a subject pool in experimental economics settings, and although several studies showed that the behavioral responses of students are largely the same as those of nonstudents in identical experiments (for a discussion see Alm, 2012; Choo et al., 2016), there is awareness that in this case, they are not taxpayers yet (Barabas and Jerit, 2010).

Practical implications

The author’s results remark the importance of increasing climate change awareness among people to let them be more willing to pay the environmental tax, for instance through investments in sensibilization campaigns on the importance of energy source usage and climate-related topic. Then, an increase in the general TMi leads to an increase in the specific (environmental) TMi. The author’s evidence showed that people with high tax morale logically recognize the positive impact of paying an environmental tax when the CCCi increases, since the more the theme becomes important, the larger the willingness to pay the specific tax. For this reason, policymakers should carry on campaigns to increase the general level of TMi to increase the overall tax compliance level and the relative tax revenues, following the guidelines given by the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (2019) to support taxpayer education programs, such as including TMi research and analysis into education programs, improving the ease of paying taxes or strengthening revenue–expenditure links to build the social contract.

Social implications

It should be paramount to increase awareness about environmental topics among people in general and among those who are relatively tax immoral. The author’s results remark on the importance of targeting energy and environmental tax policies to groups rather than to individuals. According to this evidence, we support the use of nonmonetary tools to nudge people in the environmental transition by changing their behavior in energy use, for instance through the taxation on fuel and other nonrenewable energy resources.

Originality/value

It is the first empirical study that analyzes the impact of CCCi on the environmental TMi in Italy, in particular controlling for the role of the general willingness to pay taxes (TMi). To obtain individual attitudes toward tax payment, most of the empirical studies in behavioral economics employ international surveys. For studies across citizens living in European countries, the European Social Survey (ESS) and European Values Study (EVS) represent the most used ones (see, for instance, Martinez-Vazquez and Torgler (2009) in Spain; Torgler and Werner (2005) in Germany; Nemore and Morone (2019) in Italy). However, these surveys do not allow to study the relationship between the environmental and general TMi across the same subject pool. In fact, despite the ESS (2016) provides individual responses about the willingness to pay an environmental tax, it does not provide information about the general individual attitude toward tax payment (this information is contained only in the ESS wave of 2004, hence referring to a different subject pool). On the contrary, each wave of the EVS (i.e. 2008, 2017) provides information about the general individual attitude toward tax payment, but this survey does not provide a question regarding the willingness to pay an environmental tax. Therefore, to obtain information about the willingness to pay both general and environmental taxes, across the same subject pool, it is needed to carry out a survey.

Keywords

Citation

Cascavilla, A. (2023), "Does climate change concern alter individual tax preferences? Evidence from an Italian survey", Journal of Economic Studies, Vol. 50 No. 8, pp. 1601-1617. https://doi.org/10.1108/JES-11-2022-0594

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023, Alessandro Cascavilla

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Nowadays, political agendas across governments are converging on several global-common concerns. Among others, there is the need on one hand to globally reduce CO2 emissions and on the other to increase tax compliance across both individuals and businesses. For both these two topics, insights from behavioral economics could be included and used as a tool to strengthen the policy-making process's effectiveness. Starting from tax compliance, several experimental and empirical studies found that it can be increased through policies focused on stimulating individual tax morale (TMi) (OECD, 2017). TMi refers to the intrinsic motivations of people in paying taxes (Alm and Torgler, 2006), which in turn can significantly increase overall tax compliance in a society given the evidence of a causal link between TMi and tax compliance behavior (Cummings et al., 2009; Halla, 2012). Several authors empirically showed that the TMi varies according to the sociodemographic information at the individual level (age, gender, income, employment and religiosity) as well as their economic and social preferences, such as trust in institutions, confidence in government and agreement with redistributive policies (Torgler, 2005; Alm and Torgler, 2006; Lago-Peñas and Lago-Peñas, 2010). However, different sorts of taxes can be differently perceived by taxpayers; thus TMi can vary according to the kind of tax considered within a country (Luttmer and Singhal, 2014), and this can be the case with environmental taxes (Park and Yoon, 2017).

This intuition leads us to contribute to the literature about the environmental tax morale (ETMi), namely, the individual willingness to accept an environmental tax on nonrenewable energy resources, such as fossil fuels [1]. An environmental tax can be intended as the tax rate imposed on the negative externalities coming from polluting productions (i.e. the government could set a tax in terms of euros per ton of CO2 emissions or a tax on the percentage of carbon present in nonrenewable energy resources, such as oil, gas and coal). Despite the theoretical and empirical foundations about the efficiency and effectiveness of an environmental tax, international organizations are pushing governments to impose it (UN, 2015; OECD, 2021) since it can lead to a behavioral change in both citizens and firms in the use of greener or renewable energy resources (Aldy and Stavins, 2012), in line with the UN's sustainable development goals (SDGs), in particular SDG 7 (affordable and clean energy) and SDG 13 (climate action). However, to get a visible economic and environmental impact of an environmental tax, the latter must be supported and accepted by the public. For this reason, it is paramount to understand which factors determine the individuals' level of ETMi.

The perception about environmental issues can shape the individual behavior in several contexts. According to recent studies on purchasing behavior, individuals tend to show a large willingness to pay for environmental products when they deal with decision-making (Morone et al., 2021). This larger willingness to pay for environmental and bio-based rather than conventional products is known in the literature as “green premium,” and it is particularly verified for those individuals who show a positive attitude toward environmental issues (Cheung and To, 2019). This phenomenon can be seen as a complement of the “circular premium,” introduced by D'adamo and Lupi (2021), who defined it as the difference between the circular and the normal price, which is taken into consideration in several industries and production sectors. All this recent evidence provides us the insights to investigate whether this sort of asymmetry between the standard and environmental goods can be seen also regarding the individual tax preferences. It is important to consider how individuals perceive different environmental policies, such as the introduction of carbon tax or subsidies and investments on renewable resources and which behavioral factors affect their acceptability most. According to several empirical and experimental studies, individuals tend to support more environmental subsidies than taxes (i.e. Cherry et al., 2012; Heres et al., 2015; Jagers and Hammar, 2009). This is mainly explained by the perception over the taxation system, the awareness about climate issues and the potential policy outcome such as the use of the tax revenue (i.e. Baranzini and Carattini, 2016; Douenne and Fabre, 2020).

Regarding the public acceptance of environmental taxes, Muhammad et al. (2021) carried out a review to analyze its determinants, arguing that most of the studies in this field were conducted through surveys and with experimental approaches. The most tested variables are the use of revenue, environmental attitude, political ideology, trust in the government and perceived policy effectiveness, as well as demographic traits (income, age, education and gender) obtaining mixed results. In general, it seems that people appear more willing to support a carbon tax when they (1) are aware of its efficacy and the policy content, (2) believe that the government is trustworthy, (3) have a positive attitude toward environmental protection, (4) perceive the policy is fair in terms of costs distribution and social sharing and (5) are concerned about climate change issues.

This paper focuses on the latter reason, thus on the role that the individual concern about climate change plays on the ETMi, considering the interplay with the general level of TMi. We focus our attention to the Italian case since several policies have been carried out to reduce CO2 emissions, but different concerns have prevented the introduction of a carbon tax in Italy. Among others, a relevant concern is whether Italian citizens would be willing to accept the introduction of a new tax. Although a recent work of Rotaris and Danielis (2019) showed that the potential acceptability of a carbon tax in Italy is relatively high, this topic has been scarcely explored so far. Therefore, by conducting an online survey among Italian economics students, this paper contributes to the literature by analyzing the role that the individual climate change concern (CCCi) has on the willingness to accept an environmental tax both directly and indirectly, trying to grasp how the individual general tax preferences can differ with respect to the specific (environmental) tax.

Taking as a reference the methodology to elicit the acceptance of a fossil fuel tax of recent empirical works (i.e. Fairbrother et al., 2019; Nowlin et al., 2020) we aim to demonstrate whether and how CCCi alters individual attitudes toward paying taxes, by investigating its effect on the willingness to accept an environmental tax among both TMi and tax immoral subject groups. Insights from this paper may help to understand how policymakers should design policies according to the group of individuals targeted based on their general level of TMi.

The paper is structured as follows: Section 2 describes data; Section 3 points out our research hypotheses; Section 4 deals with the description of the empirical strategy; Section 5 describes the results; finally, Section 6 concludes with some tax policy implications.

2. Data and variables

To obtain individual attitudes toward tax payment, most of the empirical studies in behavioral economics employ international surveys [2]. For studies across citizens living in European countries, the European Social Survey (ESS) and European Values Study (EVS) represent the most used ones (see, for instance, Martinez-Vazquez and Torgler (2009) in Spain; Torgler and Werner (2005) in Germany; Nemore and Morone (2019) in Italy). However, these surveys do not allow to study the relationship between the environmental and general TMi across the same subject pool. In fact, despite the ESS Round 8 (2020) provides individual responses about the willingness to pay an environmental tax, it does not provide information about the general individual attitude toward tax payment (this information is contained only in the ESS wave of 2004, hence referring to a different subject pool). On the contrary, each wave of the EVS (i.e. 2008, 2017) provides information about the general individual attitude toward tax payment, but this survey does not provide a question regarding the willingness to pay an environmental tax. Therefore, to obtain information about the willingness to pay both general and environmental taxes, across the same subject pool, it is needed to carry out a survey.

We surveyed 514 Italian university students in economics, which is the commonly subject pool in experimental economics studies, such as in tax experiments (Mascagni, 2018, p. 275). We administered the questionnaire via Google Forms, spreading it through the Instagram profile “Economia del Suicidio,” the largest social community of economics students in Italy. The sample is composed of 54.7% males and 45.3% females, with an average of 23 years old [3].

We collected information at the individual level about their perception of environmental issues, their political orientation, their economic preferences as well as their sociodemographic information. The structure of the questions that we used in the questionnaire was inspired by the ESS regarding energy use and environmental preferences and by the EVS for the individual willingness to pay taxes.

Our dependent variable is “Environmental tax morale” (ETMi), proxied by the individual answer to the question “To what extent are you in favor or against the following policies in Italy to reduce climate change?Increasing taxes on fossil fuels, such as oil, gas and coal on a 5-point Likert scale from 1, “strongly against”, to 5, “strongly in favor”. The distribution of the dependent variable is visible in the following Figure 1. The average level of ETMi is 3.71 with a standard deviation of 109.

The main independent variable is the CCCi, by which we measured with the question “How worried are you about climate change?” on a five-point Likert scale from 1, “not at all worried” to 5, “extremely worried.”

The other independent variable of interest is the general level of TMi, proxied by the question “Please tell me whether you think it can always be justified, never be justified, or something in between: Cheating on taxes if you have the chance.” Answers range from 1, “always justified”, to 10, “never justified.”

According to the literature, we accounted for several control variables (see Horodnic, 2018; Muhammad et al., 2021): trust in government, trust in politicians and political parties, political orientation (left–right), political participation, personal responsibility in combating climate change, social network activity, religiosity, age and gender. The summary of all the variables employed with their relative survey questions and descriptive statistics is reported in Table 1.

3. Research hypotheses

Building on the proposed literature and data, we formalize the following research hypotheses:

H1.

There exists a direct and positive relationship between ETMi and CCCi.

According to the literature, we expect that the more people are concerned with climate change the more they are willing to pay an environmental tax.

H2.

The ETMi positively depends on the level of individual TMi.

The expected result is that the people who are more willing to pay taxes, in general, will be also more willing to pay a specific (environmental) tax.

H3.

For individuals with high tax morale (HTM), an increase in CCCi increases the ETMi. For individuals with low TMi, the relationship between ETMi and CCCi should vanish.

We expect that an increase in CCCi should positively affect the willingness to pay an environmental tax only for those showing a higher level of general TMi. They correctly evaluate the positive externalities generated by the tax payment. Thus, with an increasing interest in a particular topic (concern about climate change), it is logical to expect that the estimated value of the positive externality generated by the tax payment on that specific topic would be positive. On the contrary, an increase in CCCi should not affect the willingness to pay an environmental tax for those who are tax immoral. In fact, given that they show low general TMi, they should not evaluate the importance of paying either a specific tax. The theoretical prediction is that given that they do not recognize the economic value of the positive externality generated by the tax payment, they would not be willing to accept an environmental tax even though they are concerned with climate change. Evidence against this hypothesis can be intended as incoherence between general and specific tax preferences (Luttmer and Singhal, 2014) which can demonstrate whether and how CCCi alters individual TMi preferences.

4. Empirical strategy

Given the ordinal distribution of our respondent variable, we estimate an ordered probit model. We start by estimating the baseline (restricted) model represented by the following equation, Equation (1):

(1)ETMi*=CCCiα+Xiβ+ui
where ETMi* represents an unobservable latent variable underlying the five-point scale measure of the ETMi of each subject i. The coefficient CCCi refers to the individual CCCi, and Xi is a vector including the control variables previously described. Finally, ui represents the error term. For the sake of robustness, we also estimate Equation (1) as a linear regression, applying the ordinary least squares (OLS) method, assuming that the dependent variable is a cardinal measure ranging from 1 to 5. Estimates are reported in Table A1 from Appendix 1.

According to the first hypothesis (H1), we expect a positive sign of the CCCis coefficient. To test our second hypothesis (H2) we extend the previous model by including as regressor the individual level of general tax morale, TMi, expecting a positive sign of the respective coefficient. In this case, TMi and ETMi could be reasonably jointly determined, since subjects who are intrinsically more willing to pay taxes in general may also be more willing to pay an environmental tax and vice versa. Therefore, in order to tackle with this potential concern, we estimate a two-stage least squares’ model by employing the second and higher moments of the potential endogenous variable as instrumental variables, following the methodology proposed by Lewbel (1997). In fact, the author demonstrates that in case of linear regressions with measurement errors, the second, third and higher moments of the potentially endogenous could represent good instruments with a two stages least squares estimates (2SLS) estimator. This approach has been widely used in empirical works (e.g. Gamso and Yuldashev, 2018; Sullivan et al., 2011). As instruments we have thus constructed the second, third and fourth moments of the TMi variable. According to this approach, the postestimation tests suggest that instruments are relevant and exogenous, and the Durbin–Wu–Hausman test suggests that the TMi variable is exogenous; hence, endogeneity unaddressed estimates can be assumed as consistent. For this reason, we report and discuss the results of the 2SLS regression in Appendix 2.

Finally, to understand the role played by the general TMi on the relationship between CCC and ETM by testing Hypothesis 3, we interact the level of CCCi with a tax morale dummy (TMD) identifying subjects with HTM. In fact, we used the TMi level as a contextual variable to split the subject pool into two subgroups: high and low TMi subjects. According to several studies about TMi, to get the respective variable, it is common to construct a dummy equal to one if the respondent declared that cheating on taxes is “never justified,” while it is zero for all the other cases (see, for instance, Torgler and Valev, 2010; Alm, 2012). This is done because with a dichotomous measure, it is possible to distinguish the group of individuals who do not justify tax evasion under any circumstances from the others (Andriani, 2016). Following this methodology, we created a TMD variable identifying those respondents who answered “never justified” to the TMi question, clustering the remaining ones in the low TMi group [4]. The HTMi group is composed by 302 subjects, while the remaining 212 subjects compose the low TMi group.

We report the distribution of the ETMi for the two groups of subjects in Figure 2. As one can see, the 29% of the HTMi group of subjects declared the largest level of ETMi, while this percentage is equal to the 20% in the low TMi group. We run some statistical tests to evaluate whether the average level of ETMi is statistically different between the two subgroups of individuals. The Mann–Whitney U test suggests that the average willingness to pay an environmental tax between high and low TMi subjects is statistically different at 5% level (z = −2.481 and p value = 0.013). The same result is given by the two-sample t test (t = −2.4 and p value = 0.008).

Once we categorized the subjects into these two categories, we construct an interaction term between the CCCi and the dummy identifying the subgroup with HTM (TMD). Hence, we run the baseline model including as regressors the TMD as well as the interaction with the CCCi (CCCixTMD). According to Hypothesis 3, we expect the interaction coefficient to be positive and statistically significant, meaning that the effect of CCCi differs between the TMi subgroups.

5. Results

This section describes the results and discusses the significance of the results. Table 2 reports the estimated coefficients and marginal effects of the restricted (Column 1), extended (Column 2) and with interaction (Column 3) models employing as the dependent variable the (ETMi).

Starting from the first column, the coefficient of CCCi is positive and statistically significant at a 1% level. This confirms the first hypothesis (H1), as already evidenced in Italy from the empirical work of Rotaris and Danielis (2019). Moreover, the individual political ideology matters: people from the right wing seem to be less willing to accept an environmental tax, and this result is in line with the conclusions of Lozza et al. (2013) who argue that left-wing taxpayers generally show higher levels of voluntary cooperation and seem to be more prone to consider tax compliance a civic duty rather than right-wing subjects. Another interesting result is that the more people trust the government the more they are willing to accept an environmental tax, and this is in line with the existing evidence (Harring and Jagers, 2013; Savin et al., 2020; Umit and Schaffer, 2020). The effect of other control variables is overall statistically negligible.

Looking at the second column, the positive and statistically significant coefficient of the TMi variable confirms the second hypothesis (H2). Also in this specification, the effect of control variables is overall consistent.

Focusing on Column 3, in line with our third hypothesis, it is visible that the coefficient of the interaction term between CCCi and TMi is positive and statistically significant at 5%. This result indicates that the concern about climate change has a diverse effect among different subgroups of people based on the level of TMi. It could play a stronger role for those individuals who declare to never justify cheating on taxes, while it is relatively weaker for those who show a lower level of general TMi.

To conclude, we summarize the following main results:

  • R1: The CCCi positively influences the ETMi.

  • R2: The general level of TMi is positively related to the willingness to pay an environmental tax.

  • R3: An increase in CCCi significantly increases the willingness to pay an environmental tax for all the individuals, although its effect may depend on the level of general TMi: it is stronger (weaker) for individuals with high (low) TMi.

6. Concluding remarks and discussion

Employing a survey among Italian economics students, this work provided innovative evidence about the differential impact of having low or HTMi on the willingness to pay an environmental tax. In line with previous evidence on both taxpayers (Muhammad et al., 2021) and consumer behavior (Morone et al., 2021), our results remark the importance of increasing climate change awareness among people, for instance through investments in sensibilization campaigns on environmental issues. Several European programs are moving in this direction. At the institutional level, in 2020, the European Commission launched the Climate Pact, which is a movement of citizens, communities and organizations that aim to mobilize people to take part in climate action to lower carbon pollution [5]. In this vein, we showed that also the trust in government and the personal political orientation matter in terms ETMi.

We further pointed out that the general level of TMi is positively related to the willingness to accept an environmental tax. Taking as reference the OECD (2019) guidelines, we advise policymakers to invest on education campaigns to raise the overall tax compliance level of citizens through the morality channel (i.e. supporting taxpayer education programs, including TMi research and analysis into education programs, improving the ease of paying taxes and strengthening revenue–expenditure links to build the social contract).

An innovative insight from this study is represented by the evidence of a different effect of CCCi depending on the individual TMi level. The results showed that there is an interaction between CCCi and TMi. This cannot be neglected in designing an effective policy aimed to increase a carbon tax's acceptability. In fact, the perceptions about climate change and tax evasion are potentially linked: the CCCi has a stronger (weaker) effect in shaping the willingness to accept an environmental tax for individuals with a high (low) morality toward their tax duties.

Although this study presented some new evidence about the linkage between climate change and tax preferences, we want to point out some limitations. Despite we employed a common methodology in this strand of research, we are aware that in an online survey individuals can be influenced by self-reporting and hypothetical choice bias (Swamy et al., 2001). Moreover, even if university students are commonly used as a subject pool in experimental studies, and although several articles showed that the behavioral responses of students are largely the same as those of nonstudents in identical experiments (for a discussion see Alm et al., 2015; Choo et al., 2016), we are aware that in this case they are not taxpayers yet (Barabas and Jerit, 2010). Therefore, we recognize the limitations for the external validity of results, which are hardly generalizable to the whole population.

Figures

Environmental tax morality (1–5) across all the samples

Figure 1

Environmental tax morality (1–5) across all the samples

Environmental tax morality (1–5) across high and low TMi subjects

Figure 2

Environmental tax morality (1–5) across high and low TMi subjects

Variables’ description and summary statistics

VariableDescriptionObs.MeanStd. dev.Min.Max.
Dependent variable
Environmental tax morale“To what extent are you in favor or against the following policies in Italy to reduce climate change? Increasing taxes on fossil fuels, such as oil, gas and coal” (1 = strongly against and 5 = strongly in favor)5143.7121.08615
Main regressors
Climate change concern“How worried are you about climate change?” (1 = not at all worried and 5 = extremely worried)5144.0230.8315
Tax morale“Cheating on taxes if you have the chance” (1 = always justified and 10 = never justified)5148.851.792110
Control variables
Trust in government“Please tell me on a score of 1–10 how much you personally trust each of the institutions. 0 means you do not trust an institution at all, and 10 means you have complete trust: Government”5144.992.337110
Political trust“Please tell me on a score of 1–10 how much you personally trust each of the institutions. 0 means you do not trust an institution at all, and 10 means you have complete trust: Political parties and politicians”5143.2222.02619
Political orientation“In politics people sometimes talk of ‘left’ and ‘right’. Where would you place yourself on this scale, where 1 means the left and 10 means the right?”5146.1852.468110
Social network activity“Have you posted or shared anything about online politics, for example on a blog, via email or on social media like Facebook or Twitter?”5140.360.4801
Political participation“Did you vote in the last national election?” (1 = yes and 0 = no)5140.6710.4701
Climate responsibility“To what extent do you feel a personal responsibility to try to reduce climate change?” (1 = not at all and 10 = a great deal)5146.2063.053110
Religiosity“How religious would you say you are?”(1 = not at all and 10 = a great deal)5142.8462.973110
GenderDummy = 1 for males5140.5470.49801
AgeAge level51422.826.191532

Results from Equation (1), ordered probit estimates

(1)(2)(3)
RestrictedExtendedWith interaction
Estimated coefficientAverage ME Estimated coefficientAverage ME Estimated coefficientAverage ME
Climate change concern (CCCi)0.426***0.125***0.422***0.124***0.471***0.136***
(0.061) (0.061) (0.138)
Tax morale (TMi) 0.055**0.016**
(0.027)
Tax morale dummy (TMD) 0.733**0.212**
CCCixTMD (0.314)
0.238**0.069**
(0.101)
Trust in government0.115***0.034***0.111***0.033***0.123***0.036***
(0.029) (0.030) (0.0295)
Political orientation−0.052**−0.015**−0.047**−0.014**−0.056***−0.016***
(0.020) (0.020) (0.020)
Political trust−0.034−0.010−0.034−0.010−0.043−0.013
(0.034) (0.034) (0.034)
Political participation−0.077−0.023−0.062−0.018−0.069−0.020
(0.111) (0.111) (0.111)
Climate responsibility−0.007−0.002−0.006−0.0020.0090.003
(0.016) (0.016) (0.017)
Social network activity−0.043−0.013−0.034−0.010−0.049−0.014
(0.101) (0.101) (0.101)
Religiosity0.0440.0130.0420.0120.0460.013
(0.101) (0.101) (0.101)
Gender0.1340.0390.167*0.049*0.1620.047
(0.099) (0.101) (0.101)
Age0.0100.0030.0080.0020.0100.003
(0.009) (0.009) (0.009)
Observations514514514514514514
Pseudo R20.062 0.065 0.061

Note(s): The standard errors in parentheses are heteroskedasticity consistent. We employ *, ** and *** to denote statistical significance at the 10, 5 and 1% levels, respectively. We report the average marginal effects for the highest score of tax morality

Results from Equation (1), OLS estimates

(1)(2)(3)
RestrictedExtendedWith interaction
Climate change concern (CCCi)0.391***0.386***0.402***
(0.055)(0.055)(0.129)
Tax morale (TMi) 0.051**
(0.025)
Tax morale dummy (TMD) 0.702**
(0.352)
CCCixTMD 0.223***
(0.095)
Trust in government0.104***0.100***0.112***
(0.027)(0.027)(0.027)
Political orientation−0.050***−0.045**−0.054***
(0.019)(0.019)(0.019)
Political trust0.0250.0260.034
(0.031)(0.031)(0.031)
Political participation−0.092−0.078−0.081
(0.104)(0.104)(0.105)
Climate responsibility0.0010.0010.015
(0.015)(0.015)(0.016)
Social network activity−0.055−0.048−0.062
(0.095)(0.095)(0.096)
Religiosity0.0250.0230.030
(0.094)(0.094)(0.095)
Gender0.1030.1330.126
(0.093)(0.094)(0.095)
Age0.0090.0080.0175
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
Constant1.827***1.341***1.571***
(0.353)(0.429)(0.480)
Observations514514514
R20.1570.1640.149

Note(s): The standard errors in parentheses are heteroskedasticity consistent. We employ *, *** and *** to denote statistical significance at the 10, 5, and 1% levels, respectively

Results from Equation (1), 2SLS with TMi as a potential endogenous variable in the extended model

Variable2SLS extended
Coeff.SE
Climate change concern0.383***(0.056)
Tax morale0.054**(0.026)
Trust in government0.105***(0.028)
Political orientation−0.044**(0.019)
Political trust−0.028(0.033)
Political participation−0.074(0.097)
Climate responsibility−0.000(0.015)
Social network activity−0.041(0.095)
Religiosity0.028(0.096)
Gender0.124(0.090)
Age0.008(0.008)
Constant1.312***(0.443)
Observations514
R20.163
Kleibergen–Paap rk LM298.261[0.000]
Kleibergen–Paap rk Wald F2316.512
Hansen J statistic1.993[0.574]
Durbin–Wu–Hausman test0.677[0.381]

Note(s): The standard errors in parentheses are heteroskedasticity consistent. We employ *, ** and *** to denote statistical significance at the 10, 5 and 1% levels, respectively. p values are presented in brackets. The TMi variable has been instrumented by its second, third and fourth moments, following the methodology of Lewbel (1997). The dependent variable is environmental tax morality on a five-point scale

First stage regression results with TMi as potential endogenous

VariablesFirst stage regression
Coeff.SE
(TMi-TM)20.437***(0.041)
(TMi-TM)30.307***(0.022)
(TMi-TM)40.034***(0.003)
Climate change concern0.029(0.040)
Trust in government0.012(0.019)
Political orientation−0.010(0.012)
Political trust−0.014(0.020)
Political participation0.033(0.066)
Climate responsibility−0.010(0.010)
Social network activity−0.071(0.060)
Gender−0.067(0.060)
Age0.001(0.004)
Religiosity0.050(0.062)
Constant8.703***(0.255)
Observations514
F test of excluded instrumentsF(3, 500) = 571.59
Prob > F = 0.000
Sanderson–Windmeijer multivariate F test:F(3, 500) = 571.59
Prob > F = 0.0000

Note(s): The standard errors in parentheses are heteroskedasticity consistent. We employ *, ** and *** to denote statistical significance at the 10, 5 and 1% levels, respectively. p values are presented in brackets. The TMi variable has been instrumented by its second, third and fourth moments, following the methodology of Lewbel (1997). The dependent variable is tax morality on a ten-point scale

Survey structure

NQuestionAnswerScale
1GenderMale; Female; Other1–3
2AgeOpen question
3Are there children/young people in your household?Yes; No; Don't know1–3
4Please indicate a score from 1 to 10. 1 means that you do not trust at all, and 10 means that you trust completely. Most peopleFrom “no trust at all” to “completely trust”1–10
5Please indicate a score from 1 to 10. 1 means that you do not trust at all, and 10 means that you trust completely. Your country's governmentFrom “no trust at all” to “completely trust”1–10
6Please indicate a score from 1 to 10. 1 means that you do not trust at all, and 10 means that you trust completely. Politicians and political partiesFrom “no trust at all” to “completely trust”1–10
7Have you posted or shared anything about politics online, for example on blogs, via email or on social media such as Facebook or Twitter?Yes; No; Don't know1–3
8In politics people sometimes talk about ‘left’ and ‘right’: where would you place yourself considering this scale, where 1 means left and 10 means right?From “left” to “right”1–10
9Would you say that it is a behavior that can always be justified, never justified or something in between that of cheating on taxes to be paid if you have the chance?From “always justified” to “never justified”1–10
10Regardless of whether you belong to a particular religion, how religious would you say you are?From “not religious at all” to “a great deal”1–10
11Overall, how confident are you that you could use less energy than you do now?From “not sure at all” to “completely sure”1–10
12You may have heard the idea that the world's climate is changing due to increases in temperature over the past 100 years. What is your opinion on this? Do you think the world's climate is changing?From “not changing at all” to “completely changing”1–5
13To what extent do you feel a personal responsibility to try to reduce climate change?From “not at all” to “a great deal”1–10
14How worried are you about climate change?From “not worried at all” to “extremely worried”1–5
15To what extent are you in favor or against the following policies in your country to reduce climate change: Increasing taxes on fossil fuels, such as oil, gas and coalFrom “strongly against” to “strongly in favor”1–5

Ordered probit estimates considering an alternative definition of tax morale

With interaction
Estimated coefficientAverage ME
Climate change concern (CCCi)0.497***0.145***
(0.127)
High tax morale (HTM) dummy0.158*0.046*
(0.091)
CCCixHTM0.139***0.040***
(0.051)
Trust in government0.118***0.035***
(0.030)
Political orientation−0.053***−0.015***
(0.020)
Political trust−0.039−0.011
(0.034)
Political participation−0.066−0.019
(0.111)
Climate responsibility0.0060.002
(0.017)
Social network activity−0.033−0.010
(0.101)
Religiosity0.0410.012
(0.101)
Gender0.186*0.054*
(0.101)
Age0.0090.003
(0.009)
Observations514514
Pseudo R20.062

Note(s): The standard errors in parentheses are heteroskedasticity consistent. We employ *, ** and *** to denote statistical significance at the 10, 5, and 1% levels, respectively. We report the average marginal effects for the highest score of tax morality

Notes

1.

With the term “environmental tax” we refer to taxes on fossil fuels, such as oil, gas and coal that may be generally intended as “carbon tax” or “Pigouvian tax.”

2.

Some examples: ESS, EVS, International Social Survey Programme (ISSP), Latinobarómetro and World Values Survey (WVS)

3.

The structure of the survey is reported in Appendix 3.

4.

For the sake of robustness, we replicate the analysis considering an alternative classification of high and low TMi subgroups. Results are reported in Appendix 4.

5.
Appendix 1

We replicated the empirical analysis by applying the OLS, obtaining the same statistical relevance of the results. Estimated coefficients are reported in the following Table A1.

Appendix 2 Robustness check

As it is visible from Table A2, the sign and the magnitude of the coefficients remained overall stable with respect to the OLS estimates. Regarding the postestimation tests, the “Kleibergen–Paap rk LM” statistic rejects the null hypothesis, suggesting the absence of an under-identification concern, while the Hansen J statistic fails to reject the exogeneity of instruments. The Kleibergen–Paap Wald F test statistic is larger than the rule of thumb of ten, suggesting that our instruments are not weak. Finally, the Durbin–Wu–Hausman test fails to reject the null hypothesis of equality between 2SLS and OLS, suggesting that the OLS estimates can be assumed as consistent.

For the sake of soundness, Table A3 below reports the results from the first stage of the 2SLS regression. All the excluded instrumental variables (second and higher moments of TMi) are jointly significant in explaining the potential endogenous variable, providing evidence that the instruments are neither weak nor under identified. Also the postestimation tests, at the bottom of the table, confirm the relevance and exogeneity of the instrumental variables.

Appendix 3 Survey structure

This appendix provides the structure of the survey that we conducted in order to carry out the analysis.

Appendix 4 Alternative classification of TMi

In this appendix we aim to evaluate whether results remain stable considering a more extreme classification of TMi. Following the methodology pointed out in Section 4, we propose an alternative classification of the two subgroups based on the level of individual TMi. To do so, inspired by Nemore and Morone (2019), we create a dummy variable that takes the value one for all those individuals showing a level of TMi larger than five (492 subjects) and zero for the others (22 subjects). In this way, we cluster individuals with very low level of TMi, and replicating the ordered probit regression with the interaction, we can evaluate whether our research hypotheses are confirmed or change considering an alternative definition of TMi. Results are reported in the following Table A5.

From the results it is visible that all the three research hypotheses are confirmed also considering a more extreme classification of TMi. Results remain overall stable and consistent with the previous ones. Despite an increase in CCCi is positively related with the ETMi, its effect may differ across groups of subjects based on their tax morality. In fact, from the interpretation of the HTM dummy coefficient, we can say that those subjects who declared higher (lower) levels of TMi are on average more (less) willing to pay an environmental tax. Moreover, the interaction between the CCCi and the dummy variable identifying HTM subjects turned out to be statistically significant. This result strengthens the fact that the CCCi plays a different role across the two subgroups.

References

Aldy, J.E. and Stavins, R.N. (2012), “The promise and problems of pricing carbon: theory and experience”, The Journal of Environment and Development, Vol. 21, pp. 152-180.

Alm, J. (2012), “Measuring, explaining, and controlling tax evasion: lessons from theory, experiments, and field studies”, International Tax and Public Finance, Vol. 19, pp. 54-77.

Alm, J., Bloomquist, K.M. and McKee, M. (2015), “On the external validity of laboratory policy experiments”, Economic Inquiry, Vol. 53 No. 2, pp. 1170-1186, 2015.

Alm, J. and Torgler, B. (2006), “Culture differences and tax morale in the United States and in Europe”, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 27, pp. 224-246.

Andriani, L. (2016), “Tax morale and prosocial behaviour: evidence from a Palestinian survey”, Cambridge Journal of Economics, Vol. 40 No. 3, pp. 821-841, May 2016, doi: 10.1093/cje/bev019.

Barabas, J. and Jerit, J. (2010), “Are survey experiments externally valid?”, American Political Science Review, Vol. 104 No. 2, pp. 226-242, doi: 10.1017/S0003055410000092.

Baranzini, A. and Carattini, S. (2016), “Effectiveness, earmarking and labeling: testing the acceptability of carbon taxes with survey data”, Environmental Economics and Policy Studies, Vol. 19, pp. 197-227.

Cherry, T.L., Kallbekken, S. and Kroll, S. (2012), “The acceptability of efficiency-enhancing environmental taxes, subsidies and regulation: an experimental investigation”, Environmental Science and Policy, Vol. 16, pp. 90-96.

Cheung, M.F.Y. and To, W.M. (2019), “An extended model of value-attitude-behavior to explain Chinese consumers' green purchase behavior”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 50, pp. 145-153.

Choo, C., Fonseca, M. and Myles, G. (2016), “Do students behave like real taxpayers in the lab? Evidence from a real effort tax compliance experiment”, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, Vol. 124, pp. 102-114, 2016.

Cummings, R.G., Martinez-Vazquez, J., McKee, M. and Torgler, B. (2009), “Tax morale affects tax compliance: evidence from surveys and an artefactual field experiment”, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, Vol. 70 No. 3, pp. 447-457.

Douenne, T. and Fabre, A. (2020), “French attitudes on climate change, carbon taxation and other climate policies”, Ecological Economics, Vol. 169, pp. 1-19.

D'Adamo, I. and Lupi, G. (2021), “Sustainability and resilience after COVID-19: a circular premium in the fashion industry”, Sustainability, Vol. 13 No. 4, p. 1861.

ESS Round 8: European Social Survey (2020), “ESS-8 2016 Documentation Report. Edition 2.2”, European Social Survey Data Archive, Sikt - Norwegian Agency for Shared Services in Education and Research, Norway for ESS ERIC, Bergen, doi: 10.21338/NSD-ESS8-2016.

Fairbrother, M., Johansson Sevä, I. and Kulin, J. (2019), “Political trust and the relationship between climate change beliefs and support for fossil fuel taxes: evidence from a survey of 23 European countries”, Global Environmental Change, Vol. 59, pp. 1-15.

Gamso, J. and Yuldashev, F. (2018), “Does rural development aid reduce international migration?”, World Development, Vol. 110, pp. 268-282.

Halla, M. (2012), “Tax morale and compliance behavior: first evidence on a causal link”, The BE Journal of Economic Analysis and Policy, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 1-27.

Harring, N. and Jagers, S.C. (2013), “Should we trust in values? Explaining public support for pro-environmental taxes”, Sustainability, Vol. 5, pp. 210-227, 2013.

Heres, D.R., Kallbekken, S. and Galarraga, I. (2015), “The role of budgetary information in the preference for externality-correcting subsidies over taxes: a lab experiment on public support”, Environmental and Resource Economics, Vol. 66, pp. 1-15.

Horodnic, I.A. (2018), “Tax morale and institutional theory: a systematic review”, International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, Vol. 38, pp. 868-886.

Jagers, S.C. and Hammar, H. (2009), “Environmental taxation for good and for bad: the effciency and legitimacy of Sweden's carbon tax”, Environmental Politics, Vol. 18, pp. 218-237.

Lago-Peñas, I. and Lago-Peñas, S. (2010), “The determinants of tax morale in comparative perspective: evidence from European countries”, European Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 26, pp. 441-453, 2010.

Lewbel, A. (1997), “Constructing instruments for regressions with measurement error when no additional data are available, with an application to patents and R&D”, Econometrica, Vol. 65 No. 5, pp. 1201-1213.

Lozza, E., Kastlunger, B., Tagliabue, S. and Kirchler, E. (2013), “The relationship between political ideology and attitudes toward tax compliance: the case of Italian taxpayers”, Journal of Social and Political Psychology, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 51-73, doi: 10.5964/jspp.v1i1.108.

Luttmer, E.F. and Singhal, M. (2014), “Tax morale”, Journal of Economic Perspectives, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 149-168.

Martinez-Vazquez, J. and Torgler, B. (2009), “The evolution of tax morale in modern Spain”, Journal of Economic Issues, Vol. 43 No. 1, pp. 1-28.

Mascagni, G. (2018), “From the lab to the field: a review of tax experiments”, Journal of Economic Surveys, Vol. 32 No. 2, pp. 273-301.

Morone, P., Caferra, R., D'Adamo, I., Falcone, P.M., Imbert, E. and Morone, A. (2021), “Consumer willingness to pay for bio-based products: do certifications matter?”, International Journal of Production Economics, Vol. 240, 108248.

Muhammad, I., Mohd Hasnu, N.N. and Ekins, P. (2021), “Review: empirical research of public acceptance on environmental tax: a systematic literature review”, Environments, Vol. 8, p. 109, 2021, doi: 10.3390/environments8100109.

Nemore, F. and Morone, A. (2019), “Public spirit on immigration issues and tax morale in Italy: an empirical investigation”, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, Vol. 81, pp. 11-18.

Nowlin, M.C., Gupta, K. and Ripberger, J.T. (2020), “Revenue use and public support for a carbon tax”, Environmental Research Letters, Vol. 15, pp. 1-11.

OECD (2017), Behavioural Insights and Public Policy: Lessons from Around the World, OECD Publishing, Paris, doi: 10.1787/9789264270480-en.

OECD (2019), Tax Morale: What Drives People and Businesses to Pay Tax?, OECD Publishing, Paris, doi: 10.1787/f3d8ea10-en.

OECD (2021), Taxing Energy Use for Sustainable Development, OECD Publishing, Paris.

Park, S.O. and Yoon, S.M. (2017), “The effects of taxpayer's environmental morale on the willingness to pay environmental tax for sustainable environment”, International Journal of Applied Engineering Research, Vol. 12 No. 24, pp. 14054-14063.

Rotaris, L. and Danielis, R. (2019), “The willingness to pay for a carbon tax in Italy”, Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, Vol. 67, pp. 659-673.

Savin, I., Drews, S., Maestre-Andrés, S. and van den Bergh, J. (2020), “Public views on carbon taxation and its fairness: a computational-linguistics analysis”, Clim. Change, Vol. 162, pp. 2107-2138, 2020.

Sullivan, P.L., Tessman, B.F. and Li, X. (2011), “US military aid and recipient state cooperation”, Foreign Policy Analysis, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 275-294.

Swamy, A., Knack, S., Lee, Y. and Azfar, O. (2001), “Gender and corruption”, Journal of Development Economics, Vol. 64 No. 1, pp. 25-55, doi: 10.1016/S0304-3878(00)00123-1.

Torgler, B. (2005), “Tax morale and direct democracy”, European Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 525-531.

Torgler, B. and Valev, N.T. (2010), “Gender and public attitudes toward corruption and tax evasion”, Contemporary Economic Policy, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 554-568.

Torgler, B. and Werner, J. (2005), “Fiscal autonomy and tax morale: evidence from Germany”, Public Finance and Management, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 423-452.

Umit, R. and Schaffer, L.M. (2020), “Attitudes towards carbon taxes across europe: the role of perceived uncertainty and self-interest”, Energy Policy, Vol. 140, pp. 1-7, 2020.

United National Framework Convention on Climate Change (2015), “Paris agreement”, available at: https://unfccc.int/sites/default/files/english_paris_agreement.pdf

Further reading

EVS (2020), “European values study longitudinal data file 1981-2008 (EVS 1981-2008). GESIS data archive, Cologne. ZA4804 Data File Version 3.1.0”, doi: 10.4232/1.13486.

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank the two anonymous referees who provided useful and detailed comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. The author would like to especially thank Prof. Andrea Morone, Prof. Jordi Ripollés, Dr. Rocco Caferra, and Dr. Jacopo Di Domenico for their valuable comments and support. The author is also grateful for the financial support from the University of Bari “Aldo Moro”.

Corresponding author

Alessandro Cascavilla can be contacted at: alessandro.cascavilla@uniba.it

Related articles